Addis Ethiopia Weblog

Ethiopia's World / የኢትዮጵያ ዓለም

  • October 2013
    M T W T F S S
     123456
    78910111213
    14151617181920
    21222324252627
    28293031  
  • Archives

  • Categories

  • Recent Posts

Archive for October 9th, 2013

High Altitude Football Teams Have Significant Advantage Over Lowland Teams

Posted by addisethiopia / አዲስ ኢትዮጵያ on October 9, 2013

ከባሕር በላይ የ 2400 ሜትር ከፍታ ባላት አዲስ አበባችን የኢትዮጵያ እግርኳስ ብሔራዊ ቡድን የየትኛውንም የአፍሪቃ ቡድን የመቅጣት ተፈጥሯዊ ዕድል አለው። ስለሆነም፡ አሰልጣኞችና የሚመለከታቸው ሁሉ ይህን ገፀበረከት በመጠቀም አስፈላጊውን የታክቲክና ስትራቴጂ እርምጃ ሊወስዱ ይገባቸዋል። በመጪው እሁድ ከናይጀሪያ ጋር በሚካሄደው ግጥሚያ የኛ ተጫዋቾች በመከላከሉ ላይ ብዙ በማተኮር አልፎ አልፎ ሹልክ እያሉ በማጥቃት ተቀናቃኞቹን በቀላሉ ብዙ እንዲሮጡ ቶሎም እንዲደክሙ ማድረግ ይቻላል ብዬ እገምታለሁ።

High Altitude Football Teams Have Significant Advantage Over Lowland Teams

FlagCrossMFootball teams from high altitude countries have a significant advantage when playing at both low and high altitudes, finds a study in this week’s Christmas issue of the BMJ.

In contrast, lowland teams are unable to acclimatise to high altitude, reducing physiological performance.

At altitude, lack of oxygen (hypoxia), cold and dehydration can lead to breathlessness, headaches, nausea, dizziness and fatigue, and possibly altitude sickness. Activities such as football can make symptoms worse, preventing players from performing at full capacity.

In May 2007, football’s governing body, the Federation of International Football Associations (FIFA), banned international matches from being played at more than 2500 m above sea level. So Patrick McSharry, a research fellow at the University of Oxford, set out to assess the effect of altitude on match results and physiological performance of a large and diverse sample of professional footballers.

He analysed the scores and results of 1,460 international football matches played at different altitudes in 10 countries in South America spanning over 100 years.

Four variables were used to calculate the effect of altitude and to control for differences in team ability (probability of a win, goals scored and conceded, and altitude difference between home and away team venues).

Altitude difference had a significant negative impact on performance. High altitude teams scored more and conceded fewer goals as altitude difference increased. Each additional 1,000m of altitude difference increased the goal difference by about half of a goal.

For example, in the case of two teams from the same altitude, the probability of the home team winning is 0.537. This rises to 0.825 for an altitude difference of 3,695m (such as high altitude Bolivia versus a sea level opponent Brazil) and falls to 0.213 when the altitude difference is -3,695m (Brazil versus Bolivia).

The surprising result is that the high altitude teams also had an advantage when playing at low altitude, so benefiting from a significant advantage over their low altitude opponents at all locations.

There is still some debate over the best strategy for low altitude teams to employ when playing at high altitude to deal with this disadvantage.

He suggests that assessing individual susceptibility to altitude illness would facilitate team selection.

Source: BMJ-British Medical Journal

Altitude in Football – How Much Difference Does it Make?

Though the debate surrounding altitude in football and sport in general has been around for a number of years, it was on April 1st 2009 that the debate was really thrown up into the air (the only pun in this piece, I promise) and into the public eye. On this date, Bolivia hosted Argentina in a World Cup qualifying match in La Paz, a city that lies approximately 3600 metres above sea level. The Bolivians came up with one of the biggest shock results in international footballing history, hammering nailed on qualifiers Argentina 6-1. The visitors looked uncharacteristically slow and lethargic, but were also uncomfortable in possession. Question the FIFA world rankings as much as one may please, but when the world number 6 ranked side at the time loses 6-1 to the ranked 58 side in such a manner, many felt this was the final straw.

However, it was two years prior that FIFA first tried to address the issue of altitude in the game. In 2007, FIFA temporarily banned all international matches that were being played at above 2,500m, but less than a year later after severe pressure from CONMEBOL the ban was repealed. In a FIFA statement from 2008 they claimed Anything between 500m and 2000m was termed “low altitude”; and at low altitudes, and at low altitudes it was claimed “minor impairment of aerobic performance becomes detectable”. Beyond this, it is unclear where the figure of 2,500m being the acceptable limit was forged, and why it was this particular altitude that was the cut off limit. It is widely known that travelling to a higher altitude without the appropriate time to acclimatise has detrimental effects on cardiovascular activities (to the extent of mountain sickness, even below altitudes of 4000m), but beyond this, higher altitudes and the respective change in atmospheric pressure can result in a change in the flight path of a football. The reason it took this long to address is relatively simple – many felt that altitude could just be part of a “home advantage”. The days of compact, dank away dressing rooms are well within memory and it is only this season that sizes of pitches have been standardised. The philosophy of complete neutrality, or in an economists terms “all other things being equal”, is actually a relatively new idea – but nevertheless an idea that has to be addressed in the modern game.

Certain difficulties arise in data collection. Simply put, the number of nations with any footballing integrity that also play at altitude is slim. More specifically, it’s three. All based in South America, the majority of studies focus on matches at Quito, Ecuador (2800m), Bogot, Colombia (2550m) and La Paz, Bolivia (3600m). This small sample size of countries over this mythical 2500m boundary provides an obstacle in data collection but luckily, all three have long standing footballing traditions – meaning many matches have taken place at all three venues. Though there have been complaints at lower levels of altitude (Denmark and Netherlands blamed poor performance on playing at Johannesburg, 1750m), many studies have scrapped the idea that altitudes below 2000m have a large effect on performance. So for the sake of argument, we shall look only at the three nations in South America.

What is immediately interesting is that is appears there is no linear correlation in the data. It isn’t a case of ‘the higher you go, the harder it gets’ – but that only after certain altitudes does the game become much harder.

AltitudeFootbal

These graphs (taken from – http://economics.mit.edu/files/6572) show an interesting finding. The dark blue represents games below 2000m and the lighter grey above 2000m. It clearly shows that whilst Bolivia and Ecuador gain is almost all areas of the game from playing at home (3600m and 2800m respectively), the lower of the three national stadiums (Colombia at 2550m) actually fared worse at home than when travelling. Furthermore, this data has been collected over many decades of play, so despite the small sample size cannot be considered an anomaly. The analysis shows that travelling teams have success in Colombia, but just 250m further up in Ecuador the winning percentage from the home team increases by 25 to 30%. Further up in La Paz, the winning percentage increase becomes 45%. Perhaps this indicates that there should be a cut off for matches at altitude, but what remains unclear is how much of this advantage comes from the altitude change and how much from other factors traditionally associated with the ‘home advantage’. The variables that naturally vary from nation to nation (humidity, atmospheric pressure, air quality, temperature etc) certainly have to factor into this advantage too.

Some studies have also, interestingly, made a connection between travelling to lower altitudes and this also having a detrimental effect on play. These studies, however, were conducted during the early part of the collective research into this subject, and failed to take into account footballing quality of the nations at higher altitudes and also the difference in quality of nations over time. Figures as erratic as a 27% less chance of winning when travelling to lower altitudes have been published – but fail to take in other basic variables; namely, that the three historically most successful teams on the continent all play at lower altitudes. If it wasn’t a complicated enough issue through battling the immeasurable variables and the limited data pool, it definitely becomes complicated once one has to sift through poor research. Any high school science or economics student will tell you that correlation does not equal causation. Reputed sports scientists working on data to be considered by FIFA, apparently, would not tell you this.

But beyond the poor quality of some studies, many are of course convincing and reputable in their argument – and it is said studies that provide some interesting findings. Initially, certain studies have claimed that whilst the outcome of the match does not change very often, the margin of winning does. In the current league table World Cup Qualifying format that South America has, this is obviously an issue that needs to be addressed. Furthermore, a different study into altitude speculates that more than a third of the goals scored in these three high altitude locations are scored in the final 15 minutes, and are overwhelmingly in favour of the home team. This could show that visiting players are able to perform at close to their optimal level, but not for a full 90 minute match.

To conclude, whether the evidence is anecdotal or statistical, the evidence is there that altitude does affect the game. However, at this time it remains unclear what specific altitudes should be deemed acceptable, but there does seem to be a presence of a certain height where players can still perform. Thus, policy implementation becomes difficult. There is a clear advantage of playing in La Paz at 3600m, and this is almost universally accepted by the scientific community looking into this matter. But below this height, the statistics often contradict each other and a ban on a single nation’s participation in their national stadium would be widely protested. Certain in game solutions have not been considered yet, however. Obviously acclimatisation is largely impractical due to the small time frame teams have to play international matches – but a system of a drinks break and short rest at the 22.5min/67.5min periods (as has been implemented in the French Ligue 1 to combat temperature) may well have a positive effect on the game’s outcome and standard over the full 90 minutes. This, at the present moment, is pure speculation on my behalf and has not been tested. What is clear is that this is an issue, and a particularly complicated one at that – no decision will come without protest, but the data shows that something definitely should be done.

Source

__

Posted in Curiosity, Ethiopia | Tagged: , , , , , , , | Leave a Comment »

Smile or Die: How Positive Thinking Fooled America and the World

Posted by addisethiopia / አዲስ ኢትዮጵያ on October 9, 2013

by Barbara Ehrenreich

Demolition of the cult of positive thinking

 

Review by Sinclair McKay

For some years we have been listening to the chilling echoes from across the Atlantic, all those life-affirming whoops and high-fives and cries of: “All right!’’ And now, as any foray into a bookshop’s “self-help’’ or “business’’ section will tell you, Britain has been turned. Corporate culture is about inane team-building exercises, “motivational speakers’’ and stifling “negative attitudes’’. Individuals are looking for “fulfilment’’ and “growth’’. It is the cult of positive thinking. And, according to Barbara Ehrenreich in this beguilingly sharp, witty and clear-eyed account, it has pretty much brought us to our knees.

Ehrenreich encountered this phenomenon in some force when she was diagnosed with breast cancer. From the mammogram on, she found that she had entered a world of pink ribbons, Ralph Lauren pink ponies and balloons, of “fun runs’’, of books featuring personal testimonies with statements like “cancer is your ticket to your real life’’ and “cancer will lead you to the divine’’. Ehrenreich found that any expression of dissent – any suggestion that one found the illness frightening, and the treatment and insurance arrangements disgraceful – led to her being howled down online by fellow sufferers for having a “bad attitude’’ and “anger and bitterness’’.

And from this, she looked around at the whole of American society, only to find that, like Invasion of the Body Snatchers, it had been taken over by the same fixed-smile think-on-the-bright-side fever. She cites the 2006 self-help multimillion bestseller The Secret – a book that claimed, among other things, that any obstacle can be overcome by the power of thought, be it obesity, myopia, or just very much wanting a beautiful necklace you have seen in a shop window. Simply thinking a thing can make it happen, or “manifest’’. The laws of the universe can be overturned on your whim.

But it’s not just individual fulfilment; the madness over there has spread to the church and to the boardroom. Ehrenreich beguilingly traces the American history of this disorder right back to “the claw marks of Calvinism’’ and the violent reaction against it. In the 1850s, a healer called Phineas Parkhurst Quimby founded the “New Thought’’ movement , teaching that “the universe was fundamentally benevolent’’ and that his patients could “leverage their own powers of mind to cure or ‘correct’ their ills’’.

Quimby inspired Mary Baker Eddy, who took this further by founding the Christian Science religion that posits, as Ehrenreich says, “that there is no material world, only Thought, Mind, Spirit, Goodness, Love… Hence there could be no such things as illness and want, except as temporary delusions.” Ehrenreich visits some modern-day Protestant mega-churches and finds, shockingly, that the message is pretty much the same. It is all soft-rock and the belief that “God wants you to prosper’’.

Ehrenreich goes on to trace how this mania for positive thinking seeped into the American boardroom, how professional management in recent years has given way to the “emotional thrills of mysticism, charisma and sudden intuitions’’, to the subculture of motivational speakers and “coaches’’.

What else was the catastrophic subprime bubble but the expression of insane, unfounded optimism? There were those at Lehman Brothers who were dismissed after raising doubts about the stability of these investments. Our very own PM’s mantra – “no more boom and bust’’ – might be said also to have been a part of this positive-thinking lunacy.

But as Ehrenreich highlights, there is also genuine cruelty in this corporate system. In “downsized’’ firms, redundant employees are told by outsourced counsellors that their job loss can be a “growth experience’’, a “step forward’’. Keep smiling – bitterness, or simply openly expressed anxiety, will make you unemployable.

And in a wider sense, this philosophy has lately been backed up by a battalion of psychologists, some of whom have formulated happiness equations and who seem a little impatient with Ehrenreich’s questions about how such equations are really quantified.

Ehrenreich throughout is bracingly acidic and engaging, but also, shall we say, surprisingly “un-negative’’ in the face of so much smiley-faced idiocy. All she is ultimately asking for in a sane world is not gloominess or pessimism but simply “vigilant realism’’. Fat chance. Because it is all really about fear in the end: fear of death, of illness, of poverty. If you believe that you fundamentally deserve to be rich and happy and healthy, the long-established religions will be of little use to you. You will instead need all the preposterous motivational New Age CDs you can get your hands on. Self-help will probably be the only growth industry of this decade.

Source

__

Posted in Life, Psychology | Tagged: , , , | 1 Comment »

 
%d bloggers like this: